Human.

Chapter 3: BIG BRAINS AND EXPANDING SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS.

43. Seyfarth, R.M., and Cheney, D.L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annual Review of Psychology 54: 14573.

44. Fitch, W.T., Neubauer, J., Herzel, H. (2002). Calls out of chaos: The adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal production. Animal Behaviour 63: 40718.

45. Mitani, J., and Nishida, T. (1993). Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 45: 73546.

46. Corballis, M.C. (1999). The gestural origins of language. American Scientist 87: 13845.

47. Rizzolatti, G., and Arbib, M.A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 21: 18894.



48. Hopkins, W.D., and Cantero, M. (2003). From hand to mouth in the evolution of language: The influence of vocal behavior on lateralized hand use in manual gestures by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental Science 6: 5561.

49. Meguerditchian, A., and Vauclair, J. (2006). Baboons communicate with their right hand. Behavioral Brain Research 171: 17074.

50. Iverson, J.M., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (1998). Why people gesture when they speak. Nature 396: 228.

51. Senghas, A. (1995). The development of Nicaraguan sign language via the language acquisition process. In MacLaughlin, D., and McEwen, S. (eds.), Proceedings of the 19th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 54352). Boston: Cascadilla Press.

52. Neville, H.J., Bavalier, D., Corina, D., Rauschecker, J., Karni, A., Lalwani, A., Braun, A., Clark, V., Jezzard, P., and Turner, R. (1998). Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: Biological constraints and effects of experience. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95: 92229.

53. Rizzolatti, G., Foga.s.si, L., and Gallese, V. (2004). Cortical mechanisms subserving object grasping, action understanding, and imitation. In Gazzaniga, M.S. (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences, vol. 3 (pp. 42740). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

54. Kurata, K., and Tanji, J. (1986). Premotor cortex neurons in macaques: Activity before distal and proximal forelimb movements. Journal of Neuroscience 6: 40311.

55. Rizzolatti, G., et al. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey, II: Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Experimental Brain Research 71: 491507.

56. Gentillucci, M., et al. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey, I: Somatotopy and the control of proximal movements. Experimental Brain Research 71: 47590.

57. Hast, M.H., et al. (1974). Cortical motor representation of the laryngeal muscles in Macaca mulatta. Brain Research 73: 22940.

58. For a review, see: Rizzolatti, G., Foga.s.si, L., and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 66170.

59. Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University.

60. Crockford, C., and Boesch, C. (2003). Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: a.n.a.lysis of barks. Animal Behaviour 66: 11525.

61. Barzini, L. (1964). The Italians. New York: Atheneum.

62. LeDoux, J.E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23: 15584.

63. LeDoux, J.E. (2003). The self: Clues from the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1001: 295304.

64. Wrangham, R., and Peterson, D. (1996). Demonic Males: Apes and the Origins of Human Violence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

65. McPhee, J. (1984). La Place de la Concorde Suisse. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.

66. Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes" Error. New York: Putnam.

67. Ridley, M. (1993). The Red Queen (p. 244). New York: Macmillan.

Chapter 3: BIG BRAINS AND EXPANDING SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS.

1. Roes, F. (1998). A conversation with George C. Williams. Natural History 107 (May): 1013.

2. Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour, I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 116 and 1752.

3. Wilson, D.S., and Wilson, E.O. (2008). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, in press.

4. Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology 46: 3537.

5. Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., and Barrett, H.C. (2005). Resolving the debate on innate ideas: Learnability constraints and the evolved interpenetration of motivational and conceptual functions. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., and Stich, S. (eds.), The Innate Mind: Structure and Content. New York: Oxford University Press.

6. Trivers, R.L., and Willard, D. (1973). Natural selection of parental ability to vary the s.e.x ratio. Science 7: 9092.

7. Clutton-Brock, T.H., and Vincent, A.C.J. (1991). s.e.xual selection and the potential reproductive rates of males and females. Nature 351: 5860.

8. Clutton-Brock, T.H. (1989) Mammalian mating systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 236: 33972.

9. Clutton-Brock, T.H. (1991). The Evolution of Parental Care. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

10. Trivers, R.L. (1972). Parental investment and s.e.xual selection. In Campbell, B. (ed.), s.e.xual Selection and the Descent of Man 18711971 (pp. 13679). Chicago: Aldine.

11. Geary, D.C. (2004). The Origin of Mind. Washington, DC: American Psychological a.s.sociation.

12. Jerrison, H.J. (1973). Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York: Academic Press.

13. Wynn, T. (1988). Tools and the evolution of human intelligence. In Byrne, W.B., and White, A. (eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

14. Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works (p. 195). New York: W.W. Norton.

15. Wrangham, R.W., and Conklin-Brittain, N. (2003). Cooking as a biological trait. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology: Part A 136: 3546.

16. Boback, S.M., c.o.x, C.L., Ott, B.D., Carmody, R., Wrangham, R.W., and Secor, S.M. (2007). Cooking and grinding reduces the cost of meat digestion. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology: Part A 148: 65156.

17. Lucas, P. (2004). Dental Functional Morphology: How Teeth Work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

18. Oka, K., Sakuarae, A., Fujise, T., Yoshimatsu, H., Sakata, T., and Nakata, M. (2003). Food texture differences affect energy metabolism in rats. Journal of Dental Research 82: 49194.

19. Broadhurst, C.L., w.a.n.g, Y., Crawford, M.A., Cunnane, S.C., Parkington, J.E., and Schmidt, W.F. (2002). Brain-specific lipids from marine, lacustrine, or terrestrial food resources: Potential impact on early African h.o.m.o sapiens. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 131B: 65373.

20. Crawford, M.A., Bloom, M., Broadhurst, C.L., Schmidt, W.F., Cunnane, S.C., Galli, C., Gehbremeskel, K., Linseisen, F., Lloyd-Smith, J., and Parkington, J. (1999). Evidence for the unique function of docosahexaenoic acid during the evolution of the modern hominid brain. Lipids 34 Suppl: S3947.

21. Broadhurst, C.L., Cunnane, S.C., and Crawford, M.A. (1998). Rift Valley lake fish and sh.e.l.lfish provided brain-specific nutrition for early h.o.m.o. British Journal of Nutrition 79: 321.

22. Carlson, B.A., and Kingston, J.D. (2007). Docosahexaenoic acid, the aquatic diet, and hominid encephalization: Difficulties in establishing evolutionary links. American Journal of Human Biology 19: 13241.

23. Byrne, R.W., and Corp, N. (2004). Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 271: 169399.

24. Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur social behaviour and primate intelligence. Science 153: 5016.

25. Humphrey, N.K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In Bateson, P.P.G., and Hinde, R.A. (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

26. Byrne, R.B., and Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

27. Alexander, R.D. (1990). How Did Humans Evolve? Reflections on the Uniquely Unique Species. Ann Arbor: Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan Special Publication No. 1.

28. Dunbar, R.I.M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6: 17890.

29. Sawaguchi, T., and Kudo, H. (1990). Neocortical development and social structure in primates. Primate 31: 28390.

30. Dunbar, R.I.M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution 22: 46993.

31. Kudo, H., and Dunbar, R.I.M. (2001). Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Animal Behaviour 62: 71122.

32. Pawlowski, B.P., Lowen, C.B., and Dunbar, R.I.M. (1998). Neocortex size, social skills and mating success in primates. Behaviour 135: 35768.

33. Lewis, K. (2001). A comparative study of primate play behaviour: Implications for the study of cognition. Folia Primatica 71: 41721.

34. Dunbar, R.I.M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 32: 16381.

35. Hill, R.A., and Dunbar, R.I.M. (2003). Social network size in humans. Human Nature 14: 5372.

36. Dunbar, R.I.M. (1996). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

37. Ben-Ze"ev, A. (1994). The vindication of gossip. In Goodman, R.F., and Ben-Ze"ev, A. (eds.), Good Gossip (pp. 1124). Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

38. Iwamoto, T., and Dunbar, R.I.M. (1983). Thermoregulation, habitat quality and the behavioural ecology of gelada baboons. Journal of Animal Ecology 52: 35766.

39. Dunbar, R.I.M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 681735.

40. Enquist, M., and Leimar, O. (1993). The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Animal Behaviour 45: 74757.

41. Kniffin, K., and Wilson, D. (2005). Utilities of gossip across organizational levels. Human Nature 16 (Autumn): 27892.

42. Emler, N. (1994). Gossip, reputation and adaptation. In Goodman, R.F., and Ben-Ze"ev, A. (eds.), Good Gossip (pp. 11738). Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

43. Taylor, G. (1994). Gossip as moral talk. In Goodman, R.F., and Ben-Ze"ev, A. (eds.), Good Gossip (pp. 3446). Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

44. Ayim, M. (1994). Knowledge through the grapevine: Gossip as inquiry. In Goodman, R.F., and Ben-Ze"ev., A. (eds.), Good Gossip (pp. 8599). Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

45. Schoeman, F. (1994). Gossip and privacy. In Goodman, R.F., and Ben-Ze"ev, A. (eds.), Good Gossip (pp. 7284). Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

46. Jaeger, M.E., Skleder, A., Rind, B., and Rosnow, R.L. (1994). Gossip, gossipers and gossipees. In Goodman, R.F., and Ben-Ze"ev, A. (eds.), Good Gossip (pp. 15468). Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.

47. Haidt, J. (2006). The Happiness Hypothesis. New York: Basic Books.

48. Dunbar, R.I.M. (1996). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

49. Brown, D.E. (1991). Human Universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.

50. Cosmides, L. (2001). El Mercurio, October 28.

51. Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2004). Social exchange: The evolutionary design of a neurocognitive system. In Gazzaniga, M.S. (ed.), Cognitive Neurosciences, vol. 3 (pp. 12951308). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

52. Stone, V.E., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Kroll, N., and Knight, R.T. (2002). Selective impairment of reasoning about social exchange in a patient with bilateral limbic system damage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99: 1153136.

53. Brosnan, S.F., and de Waal, F.B.M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425: 29799.

54. Hauser, M.D. (2000). Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think. New York: Henry Holt.

55. Chiappe, D. (2004). Cheaters are looked at longer and remembered better than cooperators in social exchange situations. Evolutionary Psychology 2: 10820.

56. Barclay, P. (2006). Reputational benefits for altruistic behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior 27: 32544.

57. Ristau, C. (1991). Aspects of the cognitive ethology of an injury-feigning bird, the piping plover. In Ristau, C.A. (ed.), Cognitive Ethology: The Minds of Other Animals. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

58. Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human by hiding. Cognition 101: 495514.

59. Dangerfield, R., in Caddyshack, Orion Pictures, 1980.

60. Tyler, J.M., and Feldman, R.S. (2004). Truth, lies, and self-presentation: How gender and antic.i.p.ated future interaction relate to deceptive behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 34: 260215.

61. Gilovich, T. (1991). How We Know What Isn"t So. New York: Macmillan.

62. Morton, J., and Johnson, M. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLEARN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychology Reviews 98: 16481.

63. Nelson, C.A. (1987). The recognition of facial expressions in the first two years of life: Mechanisms and development. Child Development 58: 899909.

64. Parr, L.A., Winslow, J.T., Hopkins, W.D., and de Waal, F.B.M. (2000). Recognizing facial cues: Individual recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology 114: 4760.

65. Burrows, A.M., Waller, B.M., Parr, L.A., and Bonar, C.J. (2006). Muscles of facial expression in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Descriptive, ecological and phylogenetic contexts. Journal of Anatomy 208: 15367.

66. Parr, L.A. (2001). Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Cognition 4: 22329.

67. For a review, see: Ekman, P. (1999) Facial expressions. In Dalgleish, T., and Power, T. (eds.), The Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (pp. 30120). Suss.e.x, UK: Wiley.

68. Ekman, P. (2002). Telling Lies: Clues to Deceit in the Marketplace, Marriage, and Politics, 3rd ed. New York: W.W. Norton.

69. Ekman, P., Friesen, W.V., and O"Sullivan, M. (1988). Smiles when lying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54: 41420.

70. Ekman, P., Friesen, W.V., and Scherer, K. (1976). Body movement and voice pitch in deceptive interaction. Semiotica 16: 2327.

71. Ekman, P. (2004). Face to face: The science of reading faces. Conversations with History (January 14).

© 2024 www.topnovel.cc